Vis enkel innførsel

dc.contributor.authorLøhre, Erik
dc.contributor.authorChandrashekar, Subramanya Prasad
dc.contributor.authorMayiwar, Lewend
dc.contributor.authorHærem, Thorvald
dc.date.accessioned2024-06-12T12:03:20Z
dc.date.available2024-06-12T12:03:20Z
dc.date.created2024-04-15T15:05:57Z
dc.date.issued2024
dc.identifier.citationJournal of Experimental Social Psychology. 2024, 113 1-13.en_US
dc.identifier.issn0022-1031
dc.identifier.urihttps://hdl.handle.net/11250/3133745
dc.description.abstractIf you are trying to persuade someone, expressing your opinion with certainty intuitively seems like a good strategy to maximize your influence. However, Karmarkar and Tormala (2010) found that the effectiveness of this tactic depends on expertise. In three experiments, Karmarkar and Tormala found support for an incongruity hypothesis, whereby non-expert sources can gain interest and influence by expressing certainty, while expert sources can increase persuasion by expressing uncertainty. In this Registered Report, we conducted a high-powered (N = 1018) direct replication of Experiment 2 by Karmarkar and Tormala (2010). In a consumer behaviour context, the original study examined whether source expertise moderated the positive effect of source certainty on the persuasive impact of a restaurant recommendation. The present replication failed to find support for the incongruity hypothesis, ηp2 = 0.00 [0.00, 0.02]: expressing certainty had a positive but non-significant effect for non-experts, d = 0.10 [−0.10, 0.34], and a positive effect for experts, d = 0.28 [0.03, 0.52]. Instead, the results supported the competing confidence heuristic hypothesis that expressed certainty would have a positive effect on persuasion, irrespective of source expertise, d = 0.18 [0.01, 0.36]. Extending the original work, we (1) controlled for the reason given for (un)certainty, and (2) examined need for closure as a potential individual difference moderator. The results indicated robust support for the confidence heuristic d = 0.25, [0.12, 0.37], but neither reason for (un)certainty nor need for closure moderated the effect as hypothesized. All materials, data, and code are available on: https://osf.io/hbjyv/.en_US
dc.language.isoengen_US
dc.publisherElsevieren_US
dc.rightsNavngivelse 4.0 Internasjonal*
dc.rights.urihttp://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/deed.no*
dc.titleUncertainty, expertise, and persuasion: A replication and extension of Karmarkar and Tormala (2010)en_US
dc.title.alternativeUncertainty, expertise, and persuasion: A replication and extension of Karmarkar and Tormala (2010)en_US
dc.typeJournal articleen_US
dc.typePeer revieweden_US
dc.description.versionpublishedVersionen_US
dc.source.pagenumber1-13en_US
dc.source.volume113en_US
dc.source.journalJournal of Experimental Social Psychologyen_US
dc.identifier.doi10.1016/j.jesp.2024.104619
dc.identifier.cristin2261775
dc.source.articlenumber104619en_US
cristin.ispublishedtrue
cristin.fulltextoriginal
cristin.qualitycode1


Tilhørende fil(er)

Thumbnail

Denne innførselen finnes i følgende samling(er)

Vis enkel innførsel

Navngivelse 4.0 Internasjonal
Med mindre annet er angitt, så er denne innførselen lisensiert som Navngivelse 4.0 Internasjonal